Motivation – Flathead Valley Community College
Posted: February 22, 2016 at 5:51 am
Motivation
Between 1924-32 the Hawthorne Works telephone equipment plant of the Western Electric Company was the site of a research program. A series of experiments focused on the levels of illumination and its affect on productivity. What made this interesting was that productivity continued to increase even when the illumination was reduced. It seemed that because the workers knew they were a part of a research they increased their productivity even when the conditions were not conducive to them. This was eventually called the Hawthorne Effect. Even though, the research that was conducted was flawed the knowledge gained on motivation was significant.
Motivation is considered goal-oriented behavior and the underlying motives. Motives arouse and direct our behavior toward some goal. There are three major categories of motives: biological, stimulus, and learned social motives.
Humor
Biological motives are things such as hunger, thirst, and physical needs.
Stimulus motives are things such as sensory stimulation, exploration, curiosity etc.
Social motives are behaviors such as achievement, power, affiliation, and other social experiences.
Motivation is behavior that is initiated and directed toward a goal, and varies in intensity and persistence.
Consider a biological motive such as hunger. There are times when you eat because its time and other times when your hunger is such you stop doing something and eat. Consider sleep, there are times when you sleep because you're tired and other times because it is time. The intensity of your behavior to eat or sleep is also, relevant. Psychologists look at motivation via behaviors associated with initiation, direction, intensity and persistence. In research we may manipulate the environment such that behaviors will or will not occur. For example, if a lab rat is not hungry it will probably not push a button to get food. Therefore it would be difficult to train a lab rat to push a button.
Theories of motivation
Genetic theories
In the late 1800's the field of psychology initially leaned toward the concept of instinct theory to explain motivation. According to instinct theory people are motivated to engage in certain behaviors because of genetic programming. However, instinct theory began to be viewed as too simplistic and was not able to explain a number of behaviors. During the 1920-30's it began to fall out of favor. Instinct theory was replaced by the concept of Drive theories. This asserted that behavior is motivated by the desire to reduce internal tension caused by unmet biological needs. When particular behavior is successful at reducing tension then it is more likely to be repeated.
The idea is these unmet biological needs drive or push us to behave in certain ways. Clark Hull and Robert Woodworth believed that drives are a triggered by the internal mechanisms of homeostasis. Homeostasis is the principle that the body monitors and maintains itself in a balance of all the systems. And the body is constantly, attempting to maintain this state. If there is loss of homeostasis then there is action to return to it. The intensity of the behavior or actions related to maintaining this balanced state are tempered with the amount of out of balance we are. When your levels of oxygen and C02 are out of balance you initiate a yawning behavior which returns the 02 and C02's balance. This is called a drive. Drive is the impulse that activates behavior to reduce a need and restore homeostasis. Again this was not adequate in explaining all behaviors and situations. Although, this was useful for understanding many types of behavior and motivation.
Incentive theories view behavior is motivated by the pull of external goals, such as rewards. During the 1950's [remember that behaviorism was considered the predominate view of behavior in psychology] psychology began to view motivation as not necessarily involving all of the internal mechanisms. The behaviorists could create or modify many behaviors by manipulating stimulus and or rewards [reinforcement].
Arousal theory
Arousal theory holds that you must consider the whole person and to understand how the person regulates his or her arousal level. Arousal is a continuum that ranges from low to high and behavior is motivated by changes in the arousal state which in turn creates behavior to efficiently address the arousal state.
Donald Hebb [1955] proposed the we are motivated to maintain an optimum level of arousal. In other words, if our arousal level drops too low we are motivated to do something about it. It is also noted that the relationship between arousal and performance is an inverted U and there is a finite point in which too much arousal will cause a deterioration in performance as well as too little arousal.
Abraham Maslow proposed the concept of "Hierarchy of Needs. Maslow was a humanist psychologist that did not discount the biological components of motivation but proposed that there were basic needs, psychological needs and self-fulfillment needs. He develop the concept of the hierarch of needs:
Physiological needs: such as food, water, warmth, rest, etc.
Safety needs: security, safety, shelter etc.
Belongingness: intimacy, friendships, social relationships
Esteem needs: prestige, feelings of accomplishment, social acceptance.
Self-actualization:achieving ones potential
Maslow approached this by studying people who where view as successful and well adjusted.
Cognitive Theory
The cognitive theorists would view motivation based on physical needs or arousal, learned behavior, and maybe non-conscious not only wants or wishes but also on how we think. Your thinking controls how you act and when you act. If you think you are in danger you will behave differently than if you don't think you are in danger. The expectancy-value theory, developed by Julian Rotter [1954] argued that behavior is the result of our expectations of achieving goals and the value that those goals have for us. Attribution is an other cognitive process related to motivation. Forces within the person [disposition forces] and forces in the environment [situation forces] interact and a person does or doesn't do goal directed behavior.
Evolutionary Psychology
Uses the principles of natural selection to study how adaptive human behaviors and psychological mechanisms have developed. Evolutionary psychology studies cross culture in an attempt to discover the commonality of human behavior.
Consider eating. There is probably many times when you eat because you are physically hungry. However, there are probably other times when you eat not because you are hungry but for something else. Have you ever over eaten? If you were motivated to eat only by the biological need to eat how would you explain eating past your hunger quenching level?
The motivation to eat
The motivation to eat is influenced by psychological, biological and/or social and cultural factors.
Biological:
Oral stimulation, stomach signals, CCK [a hormone called cholecystokinin] that acts as a neurotransmitter that signals the receptor sites in hypothalamus that are related to eating and hunger. The glucostatic theory of hunger, proposed by Jean Mayer asserted that hunger occurs when the glucose metabolism in individual cells falls below a certain level which in turn stimulates us to eat. The Lipstatic theory proposed our hunger is regulated by the storage of body fat. And that for some the storage of body fat manifests itself in over eating and obesity. Richard Nisbett suggested that there is a set-point theory of obesity. He suggests that there is a point in which the hypothalamus reacts to blood sugar level and for some this point is higher.
It has also been suggested that people who over eat may have low levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin. And msy over eat carbohydrates to compensate.
Psychological reasons to eat and over eat:
The psychodynamic view is eating is related to security, safety and maybe driven by unconscious desires and wishes. Eating maybe compensating for some unconscious psychic need. An example to this approach might suggest that a person who over eats and becomes heavy is putting a layer of fat on them to insulate them from their environment or to make him or her less attractive in order to avoid people. [this is a very simplistic manner in explaining this concept.]
The behaviorists would explain over eating as a learned behavior via direct reinforcement, imitation, and other learned behavior. And that eating is a self rewarding behavior.
The cognitive theorists would consider overeating as having its roots in the cognitive processes and that we think or believe certain things and eating is the manifestation of those cognitive processes.
Eating disorders:
Two major eating disorders are anorexia nervosa and Bulimia nervosa
Anorexia nervosa is an eating disorder in which the person becomes severely underweight because of self-imposed restrictions in eating. The APA diagnosis of this disorder includes the intense fear of being overweight and the loss of 15% of body weight without any physical problems that would account for it. Although both sexes are represented in this disorder more females present with this disorder. It is estimated 1% of the female teenage population is affected by anorexia. The person literally starves themselves, often times, to death. The anorexic will either ignore or is not aware that they are not ingesting enough to maintain a healthy state. Their self-perceptions are extremely disoriented. They view themselves as appearing differently than others. When they look in the mirror they appear to not see themselves as others do. They see fat where there is none, they a poor at self monitoring and evaluation. Causes seem to be related to societal pressures to be thin as well as culture as that it is a disorder that is mainly in the Western Americian/Europeon culture.
Bulimia nervosa is a eating disorder that is manifested by eating large amounts of calorie rich food in a short time then purging the food by vomiting or using laxatives. This is contrasted to anorexia as often individuals with bulimia are within normal weight ranges. Sometimes the bulimic will ingest as much as 50,000 calories at one time in a short period. There are periods of binge eating which occur in secret. As with anorexia, cultural pressures seemed to be related as well as faulty thinking about food intake, distorted body perception and a tendency toward perfectionism. Also, worth noting is there seems to be genetic factors related as well.
Curiosity and exploration
The curiosity motive causes us to seek out a certain amount of novelty and complexity, and with not apparent motivation, we seek out and explore new environments. Young children prefer complex patterns over simple ones.
Some of the research suggests that our level of curiosity motive stays stable throughout our lifetime. The motivational theory of competence state that we are motivated to interact successfully with our environment.
Intrinsic motivation is behavior that results from interpersonal factors.
Extrinsic motivation is behavior that results for extra-personal factors. [what we can get]
Edward Deci [1975] proposed his theory of intrinsic motivation. He suggested that we need to feel competent in controlling and interacting with our environment. And self-determination is can increase intrinsic motivated behavior.
Learned Social Motives
Henry Murray [1938] contended that social motives are largely learned and he developed a list of basic social motives:
Achievement
Affiliation
Aggression
Autonomy
Dominance
Nurturance
Play
Understanding
He and others suggest we are taught to maintain a certain level of the above. Further, we are taught and acquire behavior that allows us to achieve our learned social motives.
The level of intensity by which the person attempts to meet the learned social motives is determined both by individual and familial influences as well as by cultural and societal influences.
Research of fear of failure indicates that people who score low on the need for achievement tests also tend to have a high fear of failure.
Also, it has been shown that the need for affiliation increases during period of anxiety. Misery needs company. When given the choice of waiting in an anxious situation in a nice environment and alone or in a poor environment with others most would choose the later.
Return
Sources:
Psychology a Connectext 4th edition, Terry F. Pettijohn
Discovering Psychology, Don Hockenbury & Sandra Hockenbury
Social Psychology, 5th edition, Deaux Wrightsman
Motivation – Analytic Technologies
Posted: at 5:51 am
Overview
At a simple level, it seems obvious that people do things, such as go to work, in order to get stuff they want and to avoid stuff they don't want.
Why exactly they want what they do and don't want what they don't is still something a mystery. It's a black box and it hasn't been fully penetrated.
Overall, the basic perspective on motivation looks something like this:
In other words, you have certain needs or wants (these terms will be used interchangeably), and this causes you to do certain things (behavior), which satisfy those needs (satisfaction), and this can then change which needs/wants are primary (either intensifying certain ones, or allowing you to move on to other ones).
A variation on this model, particularly appropriate from an experimenter's or manager's point of view, would be to add a box labeled "reward" between "behavior" and "satisfaction". So that subjects (or employees), who have certain needs do certain things (behavior), which then get them rewards set up by the experimenter or manager (such as raises or bonuses), which satisfy the needs, and so on.
People seem to have different wants. This is fortunate, because in markets this creates the very desirable situation where, because you value stuff that I have but you don't, and I value stuff that you have that I don't, we can trade in such a way that we are both happier as a result.
But it also means we need to try to get a handle on the whole variety of needs and who has them in order to begin to understand how to design organizations that maximize productivity.
Part of what a theory of motivation tries to do is explain and predict who has which wants. This turns out to be exceedingly difficult.
Many theories posit a hierarchy of needs, in which the needs at the bottom are the most urgent and need to be satisfied before attention can be paid to the others.
Maslow's hierarchy of need categories is the most famous example:
Specific examples of these types are given below, in both the work and home context. (Some of the instances, like "education" are actually satisfiers of the need.)
According to Maslow, lower needs take priority. They must be fulfilled before the others are activated. There is some basic common sense here -- it's pointless to worry about whether a given color looks good on you when you are dying of starvation, or being threatened with your life. There are some basic things that take precedence over all else.
Or at least logically should, if people were rational. But is that a safe assumption? According to the theory, if you are hungry and have inadequate shelter, you won't go to church. Can't do the higher things until you have the lower things. But the poor tend to be more religious than the rich. Both within a given culture, and across nations.So the theory makes the wrong prediction here.
Or take education: how often do you hear "I can't go to class today, I haven't had sex in three days!"? Do all physiological needs including sex have to be satisfied before "higher" needs? (Besides, wouldn't the authors of the Kama Sutra argue that sex was a kind of self-expression more like art than a physiological need? that would put it in the self-actualization box).Again, the theory doesn't seem to predict correctly.
Cultural critique: Does Maslow's classification really reflect the order in which needs are satisfied, or is it more about classifying needs from a kind of "tastefulness" perspective, with lofty goals like personal growth and creativity at the top, and "base" instincts like sex and hunger at the bottom? And is self-actualization actually a fundamental need? Or just something that can be done if you have the leisure time?
Alderfer classifies needs into three categories, also ordered hierarchically:
This is very similar to Maslow -- can be seen as just collapsing into three tiers. But maybe a bit more rational. For example, in Alderfer's model, sex does not need to be in the bottom category as it is in Maslow's model, since it is not crucial to (the individual's) existence. (Remember, this about individual motivation, not species' survival.) So by moving sex, this theory does not predict that people have to have sex before they can think about going to school, like Maslow's theory does.
Alderfer believed that as you start satisfying higher needs, they become more intense (e.g., the power you get the more you want power), like an addiction.
Do any of these theories have anything useful to say for managing businesses? Well, if true, they suggest that
Some needs are acquired as a result of life experiences
Again similar to maslow and alderfer.
These needs can be measured using the TAT (thematic apperception test), which is a projection-style test based on interpreting stories that people tell about a set of pictures.
This theory suggests that there are actually two motivation systems: intrinsic and extrinsic that correspond to two kinds of motivators:
One or the other of these may be a more powerful motivator for a given individual.
Intrinsically motivated individuals perform for their own achievement and satisfaction. If they come to believe that they are doing some job because of the pay or the working conditions or some other extrinsic reason, they begin to lose motivation.
The belief is that the presence of powerful extrinsic motivators can actually reduce a person's intrinsic motivation, particularly if the extrinsic motivators are perceived by the person to be controlled by people. In other words, a boss who is always dangling this reward or that stick will turn off the intrinsically motivated people.
Note that the intrinsic motivators tend to be higher on the Maslow hierarchy.
According to Herzberg, two kinds of factors affect motivation, and they do it in different ways:
So hygiene factors determine dissatisfaction, and motivators determine satisfaction. The two scales are independent, and you can be high on both.
Suppose employee A gets a 20% raise and employee B gets a 10% raise. Will both be motivated as a result? Will A be twice as motivated? Will be B be negatively motivated?
Equity theory says that it is not the actual reward that motivates, but the perception, and the perception is based not on the reward in isolation, but in comparison with the efforts that went into getting it, and the rewards and efforts of others. If everyone got a 5% raise, B is likely to feel quite pleased with her raise, even if she worked harder than everyone else. But if A got an even higher raise, B perceives that she worked just as hard as A, she will be unhappy.
In other words, people's motivation results from a ratio of ratios: a person compares the ratio of reward to effort with the comparable ratio of reward to effort that they think others are getting.
Of course, in terms of actually predicting how a person will react to a given motivator, this will get pretty complicated:
Reinforcement Theory
Operant Conditioning is the term used by B.F. Skinner to describe the effects of the consequences of a particular behavior on the future occurrence of that behavior. There are four types of Operant Conditioning: Positive Reinforcement, Negative Reinforcement, Punishment, and Extinction. Both Positive and Negative Reinforcement strengthen behavior while both Punishment and Extinction weaken behavior.
Reinforcement schedules.
The traditional reinforcement schedule is called a continuous reinforcement schedule. Each time the correct behavior is performed it gets reinforced.
Then there is what we call an intermittent reinforcement schedule. There are fixed and variable categories.
The Fixed Interval Schedule is where reinforcement is only given after a certain amount of time has elapsed. So, if you decided on a 5 second interval then each reinforcement would occur at the fixed time of every 5 seconds.
The Fixed Ratio Schedule is where the reinforcement is given only after a predetermined number of responses. This is often seen in behavior chains where a number of behaviors have to occur for reinforcement to occur.
The Variable Interval Schedule is where the reinforcement is given after varying amounts of time between each reinforcement.
The Variable Ratio Schedule is where the reinforcement is given after a varying number of correct responses.
Fluctuating combinations of primary and secondary reinforcers fall under other terms in the variable ratio schedule; For example, Reinforcers delivered Intermittently in a Randomized Order (RIR) or Variable Ratio with Reinforcement Variety (VRRV).
(yearly raise)
[short term]
(unexpected bonus based on merit)
[medium term]
(commissions or piecework pay)
[medium term]
(team-based bonus)
[long term]
This theory is meant to bring together many of the elements of previous theories. It combines the perceptual aspects of equity theory with the behavioral aspects of the other theories. Basically, it comes down to this "equation":
M = E*I*V
or
motivation = expectancy * instrumentality * valence
M (motivation) is the amount a person will be motivated by the situation they find themselves in. It is a function of the following.
E (expectancy) = The person's perception that effort will result in performance. In other words, the person's assessment of the degree to which effort actually correlates with performance.
I (instrumentality) = The person's perception that performance will be rewarded/punished. I.e., the person's assessment of how well the amount of reward correlates with the quality of performance. (Note here that the model is phrased in terms of extrinsic motivation, in that it asks 'what are the chances I'm going to get rewarded if I do good job?'. But for intrinsic situations, we can think of this as asking 'how good will I feel if I can pull this off?').
V(valence) = The perceived strength of the reward or punishment that will result from the performance. If the reward is small, the motivation will be small, even if expectancy and instrumentality are both perfect (high).
Here is the original post:
Motivation - Analytic Technologies
Motivation – EduTech Wiki
Posted: at 5:51 am
This article or chapter is incomplete and its contents need further attention. Some information may be missing or may be wrong, spelling and grammar may have to be improved, use your judgment!
This article should be rewritten. Only use it to grab some ideas and pointers to further reading ... DSchneider
Often, on makes a distinction between intrinsic motivation (desire to do something for personal, internal reasons) and extrinsic motivation (seek recompensation and avoid punishment), but the two are very much linked and difficult to separate.
See also: Affect, self-efficacy theory, flow theory, student satisfaction
Maslow (1954) wrote one of most cited articles on lower-level human motivation and that was based on a synthesis of the state of art. He later expaned it to include higher level needs.
According to Huitt (2001), Maslow's model broke down human motivation into a hierarchy of needs
This hierarchy is a bottom up structure with need (1) being the lowest level. Each lower need must be met before one can move to the next level
Drive theories are behaviorist and/or cognitivist in nature and explain behavior as as response to psychological and social needs. The relation between need and motivation can be described as a feedback system. The bigger the need the bigger motivation and the lesser the need, the lesser the motivation. E.g. We are more motivated to eat when we hungry and less motivated after we have eaten. Regarding instruction, learners in these models are rather passive and the environment (materials, teachers, etc.) in control.
Needs are cognitively elaborated into concrete motivational goals and means-end structures. Being motivated means striving for goals which are by definition not yet realized at the moment that they are formulated or expresses (Nuttin, 1980). The individuals hopes and expects to reach them at a certain moment in time as a consequence of his actions. There are three degrees of activation: (1) passive action to respond to stimulus, (2) respond actively to selected stimuli and (3) change the environment. At levels two and three, goals and the anticipated outcome are the source of motivation. Good goals are: clear, personally relevant, proximal, progress can be seen, interim successes are possible.
One can distinguish between learning goals (desire to be able to master a task) and performance goals (desire to appear competent or at least better than the others).
For Greenwald (1982), the 'self' becomes involved in an activity in three conditions:
Herzberg et al. (1959) identified two main factors of work satisfaction: hygiene needs, which are influenced by the physical and psychological conditions in which people work, and motivator needs, which Herzberg described as being very similar to the higher-order needs in Maslows (1954) hierarchy theory. (Furnham et al., 2009). Fullfillment of hygiene needs only can eliminate dissatisfaction but it can't motivate. So let's let just look at motivation. F. Herzberg's 5 factors motivation/demotivation model is based on the idea that motivation is related to internal satisfaction dependent on external factors:
This model was extracted from fourteen first-level job factors related with job satisfaction and dissatisfaction: achievement, recognition, the work itself, responsibility, possibility of advancement, possibility of growth, salary status, the quality of interpersonal relations with superiors, the quality of interpersonal relations with peers, technical supervision, agreement with company policies and administration, pleasant working conditions, external factors from personal life, and job security. The five factor model defines motivation as a result of the job content (or the learning activity if we dare to extrapolate).
In a more recent publication, Furnham et al (1999) relate work motivation to personality. E.g. they found that extraversion can be related to a preference for Herzberg's motivator factors, and neuroticism a preference for hygiene factors.
Warr (????) describes motivation as a cognitive process that concerns future planned actions and that can be based on a set of reasons:
A similar model concerns the characteristics of the task and the job environment (Hackman and Oldman, 1976): Factors influencing motivation are:
These theories had important impact on how work should be designed. In simple terms, work is more motivating if tasks are varied and meaningful, if the worker can exercise control. On the other hand task also should lead to results that are acknowledged by the environment. According to many studies, these models don't work for everyone but best for people who do have "growth needs".
How does the actor/learner explain what happens to him? How does he explain the outcome (e.g. success and failure). Dimensions of causes that learners attribute can be:
Of course the pedagogical design and the teacher can influence these perceptions. In empirical studies these factors also show up in combinations related to given issues and affect. Philipp Dessus (2001) summarizes Crahay, 1999, p. 284 and Archambault & Chouinard, 1996, p. 110):
Motivation equals the degree of perceived control. Influencing factors are: choices, impositions by others through threats or controlling statements or being watched, getting feedback, getting extrinsic rewards
Most authors agree that immediate extrinsic rewards don't contribute much to motivation. Long term extrinsic rewards do play a role. E.g. studies about job satisfaction (e.g. Herzberg et al, 1969) identify recognition for achievement and social progression as critical factors that could be called extrinsic. Achievement, work itself and responsability are rather intrinsic.
The informal management literature often makes similare statements. E.g. Rewards and punishments are not opposites at all; they are two sides of the same coin. And it is a coin that does not buy very much (Kohn, 1999). Or more explicitely Drawing from hundreds of studies, Kohn demonstrates that people actually do inferior work when they are enticed with money, grades, or other incentives. Programs that use rewards to change people's behavior are similarly ineffective over the long run. Promising goodies to children for good behavior can never produce anything more than temporary obedience. In fact, the more we use artificial inducements to motivate people, the more they lose interest in what we're bribing them to do. Rewards turn play into work, and work into drudgery. (Punished by Rewards). Daniel H. Pink's (2010) Drive book makes similar statements.
Konrad (2005) argues that in an educational context we should complete sociological, psychological and interactional models with a decision making approach where learners are seen as decision makers, who more or less consciously analyse their past experiences, current life and work situation, and future expectations, and base their decisions to participate or not on these complex elements which form the motivation structure. (Konrad, 2005:7).
Manninen (2004:4) also points out that different stages of the learning process may engage different motivations. Motives activate, direct and maintain the learning activity (Ruohotie 2000, 8). Therefore motivation and its factors should be defined keeping in mind in which part of the process they belong. For example, activating elements are more general factors which are more or less stable personality elements (like curiosity, learner self-image), while directing factors (like outcome beliefs, task value) focus the persons interest on a specific target (learning activity). Elements maintaining the motivation, on the other hand, influence learning activity while it is taking place (during the training programme; test anxiety, expectancy for success) or as feedback loop after the learning experience (like achievements) influencing therefore future motivation to participate or not.
The following table lists some non exclusive models of explanation. It is based on Manninen (2004) and was also found in Konrad (2005):
In any case, DSchneider believes that motivation in relation to teaching and learning has to be conceived as multi-dimensional phenomenon (construct) influenced by various variables such as:
Motivation always has been a key variable in education:
In general, explanations regarding the source(s) of motivation can be categorized as either extrinsic (outside the person) or intrinsic (internal to the person). Intrinsic sources and corresponding theories can be further subcategorized as either body/physical, mind/mental (i.e., cognitive, affective, conative) or transpersonal/spiritual. (Huitt, 2001).
Motivation to learn according to Huitt, 2001 (reproduced without asking permission)
According to Marilla Svinicki, teachers, in order to increase student motivation, should:
How theories are used to design for motivation
Guerrero & Sire (2000:3-4) again point out the complexity of motivation and single out self-efficacy and instrumentality as two key dimensions to study training motivation of french workers.
One of the definitions widely used in recent studies of training motivation (Baldwin et al., 1991; Facteau et al., 1995; Quinones, 1995) is that introduced by Noe in 1986 in the Academy of Management Review. It is inspired by American research on motivation at work (Campbell & Pritchard, 1976). Training motivation is described as "a specific desire of the trainee to learn the content of the training program". Other definitions refer to the effort exerted in training to learn the course contents (Hicks & Klimoski, 1987), along with Vroom's expectancies theory (1964). Accordingly, Mathieu et al. (1992) describe training motivation as "trainees' perceptions that doing well in a program would lead to better job performance and consequently to valued outcomes." Furthermore, several concepts have been used to describe training motivation. In addition to expectancies theories (Vroom 1964, Porter and Lawler 1968), authors have built upon the studies of Bandura (1977) on self-efficacy and Adams on equity (1965).
Manninen (2004:3) points out that The key question (why adults engage themselves in learning activities and why not?) can be analysed using two different but overlapping theoretical frameworks, which are theories of motivation and theories of participation. Participation [...] deals with the processes which make people to participate in organised training situations. In addition to psychological explanations (cf. Boshier, see Garrison 1987), there has been more sociologically oriented (Lehtonen & Tuomisto 1972) and interaction models (Rubenson 1979) which take into account the wider context and individual life situations.
In a large-scale empirical studies Manninen & Birke (2005) and Konrad (2005) studied the learning motivations of lower qualified workers using a multi-dimensional learning motivation scale that was based on the following concepts (categories). Values, i.e Alpha, Neutral, Beta refer to Dynamic concept analysis (Kontinen, 2002).
Results let to a typology of motivation according to 2 dimensions: Simple work/complex work situation and low/high motivation.
An important finding from this study was that Learning preferences identified by lower qualified workers across the countries (like learning by doing & learning from others) indicate that theoretical models such as shared expertise and cognitive apprenticeship (Bereiter & Scardamalia 1993; Lave & Wenger 1991) are suitable for this target group. (Konrad, 2005:22). The author also makes a connection to constructivism, i.e. knowledge as direct and social experience, and citing Resnik (1991) social processes as cognition.
Thus, when an individual joins an existing group of competent practitioners, they are motivated by membership of that group both to strengthen their identity as learners and, at least as importantly, to promote the success of the group. This process of mastering the virtuous circle of learning to learn is a central part of the process of successful adult learning. In a structured workplace, the role of the competent members is crucial, whether those with formal status (such as supervisors) or as informal leaders.
In practical terms this means that situated motivation will be enhanced by the motivating potential of the instructional design. Adler (2001) suggests:
Motivation is linked to emotions, but emotions an related emotional design of instruction are yet another category of interest: Here is a citation from Astleitner (2000:169): "It is well known in the field of basic and applied research on education and psychology that cognitive, motivational, and emotional processes are related to the world in different ways. Cognitive processes concern the acquisition and representation of knowledge and have a representative relation to the world of objects and facts. Motivational processes refer to goal states of the organism and have an actional relation to the world. Emotional processes are based on the acceptance or rejection of objects and facts and have an evaluational relation to the world (Kuhl, 1986)"
(To do)
Jean-Louis Berger, Stuart A. Karabenick, Motivation and students use of learning strategies: Evidence of unidirectional effects in mathematics classrooms, Learning and Instruction, Volume 21, Issue 3, June 2011, Pages 416-428, ISSN 0959-4752, 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.06.002.
(http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0191886998002025)
Read the original:
Motivation - EduTech Wiki
6 Types of Motivation Explained – Tips for Life
Posted: at 5:51 am
What makes people do what they do? Why do some people succeed while others fail? The answer just might be motivation. We know that from an early age motivation prompts us to want to learn and exhibit different types of behavior and stimulates us to accomplish new feats of success. As we grow and mature through the different stages of our lives, we hopefully learn what motivates us and what does not.
Motivation is generally defined as the force that compels us to action. It drives us to work hard and pushes us to succeed. Motivation influences our behavior and our ability to accomplish goals.
There are many different forms of motivation. Each one influences behavior in its own unique way. No single type of motivation works for everyone. Peoples personalities vary and so accordingly does the type of motivation, that is most effective at inspiring their conduct.
A form of motivation that involves rewards, both monetary and nonmonetary is often called incentive motivation. Many people are driven by the knowledge that they will be rewarded in some manner for achieving a certain target or goal. Bonuses and promotions are good examples of the type of incentives that are used for motivation.
Fear motivation involves consequences. This type of motivation is often one that is utilized when incentive motivation fails. In a business style of motivation often referred to as the, carrot and stick, incentive is the carrot and fear is the stick.
Punishment or negative consequences are a form of fear motivation. This type of motivation is commonly used to motivate students in the education systemand also frequently in a professional setting to motivate employees. If we break the rules or fail to achieve the set goal, we are penalized in some way.
Achievement motivation is also commonly referred to as the drive for competency. We are driven to achieve goals and tackle new challenges. We desire to improve skills and prove our competency both to others and to ourselves. Generally, this feeling of accomplishment and achievement is intrinsic in nature.
However, in certain circumstances be motivation for achievement may involve external recognition. We often have a desire or need to receive positive feedback from both our peers and our superiors. This may include anything from an award to a simple pat on the back for a job well done.
The need for self-improvement is truly an internal motivation. A burning desire to increase our knowledge of ourselves and of the outside world can be a very strong form of motivation. We seek to learn and grow as individuals.
Motivation for growth can also be seen in our yearning for change. Many of us are wired by our personality or upbringing to constantly seek a change in either our external or internal environment or knowledge. We view stagnation to be both negative and undesirable.
The motivation of power can either take the form of a desire for autonomy or other desire to control others around us. We want to have choices and control over our own lives. We strive for the ability to direct the manner in which we live now and the way our lives will unfold in the future.
We also often aspire to control others around us. The desire for control is stronger in some people than others. In some cases, the craving for power induces people to harmful, immoral, or illegal behavior. In other situations, the longing for power is merely a desire to affect the behavior of others. We simply want people to do what we want, according to our timetable, and the way we want it done.
Many people are motivated by social factors. This may be a desire to belong and to be accepted by a specific peer group or a desire to relate to the people in our sphere or in the larger world. We have an innate need to feel a connection with others. We also have the need for acceptance and affiliation.
A genuine and passionate desire to contribute and to make a difference in the lives of others can be another form of social motivation. If we have a longing to make a contribution to the world around us, it is generally a sign that we are motivated by social factors.
The real importance of understanding the different types of motivation is in our ability to determine which form of motivation is the most effective for inspiring the desired behavior in either others or ourselves. None of these styles of motivation is inherently good or bad, the positive or negative outcome is truly determined by the way they are used.
(Photo credit: Businessman Placing Motivation via Shutterstock)
Teaching Concepts: Motivation Cengage Learning
Posted: at 5:51 am
Motivation
Excerpted from Chapter 11 of Biehler/Snowman, PSYCHOLOGY APPLIED TO TEACHING, 8/e, Houghton Mifflin, 1997.
Definition of Motivation (p. 399)
Behavioral Views of Motivation (pp. 399-402)
Cognitive Views of Motivation (pp. 402-406)
The Humanistic View of Motivation (pp. 406-409)
The Impact of Cooperative Learning on Motivation (pp. 416-417)
Suggestions for Teaching in Your Classroom: Motivating Students to Learn (p. 422)
Resources for Further Investigation (pp. 433-434)
Definition of Motivation
Motivation is typically defined as the forces that account for the arousal, selection, direction, and continuation of behavior. Nevertheless, many teachers have at least two major misconceptions about motivation that prevent them from using this concept with maximum effectiveness. One misconception is that some students are unmotivated. Strictly speaking, that is not an accurate statement. As long as a student chooses goals and expends a certain amount of effort to achieve them, he is, by definition, motivated. What teachers really mean is that students are not motivated to behave in the way teachers would like them to behave. The second misconception is that one person can directly motivate another. This view is inaccurate because motivation comes from within a person. What you can do, with the help of the various motivation theories discussed in this chapter, is create the circumstances that influence students to do what you want them to do.
Many factors determine whether the students in your classes will be motivated or not motivated to learn. You should not be surprised to discover that no single theoretical interpretation of motivation explains all aspects of student interest or lack of it. Different theoretical interpretations do, however, shed light on why some students in a given learning situation are more likely to want to learn than others. Furthermore, each theoretical interpretation can serve as the basis for the development of techniques for motivating students in the classroom. Several theoretical interpretations of motivation -- some of which are derived from discussions of learning presented earlier -- will now be summarized.
Top
Behavioral Views of Motivation
Operant Conditioning and Social Learning Theory
The Effect of Reinforcement In Chapter 8 we discussed Skinner's emphasis of the role of reinforcement in learning. After demonstrating that organisms tend to repeat actions that are reinforced and that behavior can be shaped by reinforcement, Skinner developed the technique of programmed instruction to make it possible for students to be reinforced for every correct response. According to Skinner, supplying the correct answer--and being informed by the program that it is the correct answer--motivates the student to go on to the next frame; and as the student works through the program, the desired terminal behavior is progressively shaped.
Following Skinner's lead, many behavioral learning theorists devised techniques of behavior modification on the assumption that students are motivated to complete a task by being promised a reward of some kind. Many times the reward takes the form of praise or a grade. Sometimes it is a token that can be traded in for some desired object; and at other times the reward may be the privilege of engaging in a self-selected activity.
Operant conditioning interpretations of learning may help reveal why some students react favorably to particular subjects and dislike others. For instance, some students may enter a required math class with a feeling of delight, while others may feel that they have been sentenced to prison. Skinner suggests that such differences can be traced to past experiences. He would argue that the student who loves math has been shaped to respond that way by a series of positive experiences with math. The math hater, in contrast, may have suffered a series of negative experiences.
The Power of Persuasive Models Social learning theorists, such as Albert Bandura, call attention to the importance of observation, imitation, and vicarious reinforcement (expecting to receive the same reinforcer that we see someone else get for exhibiting a particular behavior). A student who identifies with and admires a teacher of a particular subject may work hard partly to please the admired individual and partly to try becoming like that individual. A student who observes an older brother or sister reaping benefits from earning high grades may strive to do the same with the expectation of experiencing the same or similar benefits. A student who notices that a classmate receives praise from the teacher after acting in a certain way may decide to imitate such behavior to win similar rewards. As we pointed out in Chapter 8, both vicarious reinforcement and direct reinforcement can raise an individual's sense of self-efficacy for a particular task, which, in turn, leads to higher levels of motivation.
Top
Cognitive Views of Motivation
Cognitive views stress that human behavior is influenced by the way people think about themselves and their environment. The direction that behavior takes can be explained by four influences: the inherent need to construct an organized and logically consistent knowledge base, one's expectations for successfully completing a task, the factors that one believes account for success and failure, and one's beliefs about the nature of cognitive ability.
The Impact of Cognitive Development
This view is based on Jean Piaget's principles of equilibration, assimilation, accommodation, and schema formation. Piaget proposes that children possess an inherent desire to maintain a sense of organization and balance in their conception of the world (equilibration). A sense of equilibration may be experienced if a child assimilates a new experience by relating it to an existing scheme, or the child may accommodate by modifying an existing scheme if the new experience is too different.
In addition, individuals will repeatedly use new schemes because of an inherent desire to master their environment. This explains why young children can, with no loss of enthusiasm, sing the same song, tell the same story, and play the same game over and over and why they repeatedly open and shut doors to rooms and cupboards with no seeming purpose. It also explains why older children take great delight in collecting and organizing almost everything they can get their hands on and why adolescents who have begun to attain formal operational thinking will argue incessantly about all the unfairness in the world and how it can be eliminated (Stipek, 1993).
Top
The Need for Achievement
Have you ever decided to take on a moderately difficult task (like take a course on astronomy even though you are a history major and have only a limited background in science) and then found that you had somewhat conflicting feelings about it? On the one hand, you felt eager to start the course, confident that you would be pleased with your performance. But on the other hand, you also felt a bit of anxiety because of the small possibility of failure. Now try to imagine the opposite situation. In reaction to a suggestion to take a course outside your major, you flat out refuse because the probability of failure seems great, while the probability of success seems quite small.
In the early 1960s John Atkinson (1964) proposed that such differences in achievement behavior are due to differences in something called the need for achievement. Atkinson described this need as a global, generalized desire to attain goals that require some degree of competence. He saw this need as being partly innate and partly the result of experience. Individuals with a high need for achievement have a stronger expectation of success than they do a fear of failure for most tasks and therefore anticipate a feeling of pride in accomplishment. When given a choice, high-need achievers seek out moderately challenging tasks because they offer an optimal balance between challenge and expected success. By contrast, individuals with a low need for achievement avoid such tasks because their fear of failure greatly outweighs their expectation of success, and they therefore anticipate feelings of shame. When faced with a choice, they typically opt either for relatively easy tasks because the probability of success is high or rather difficult tasks because there is no shame in failing to achieve a lofty goal.
Atkinson's point about taking fear of failure into account in arranging learning experiences has been made more recently by William Glasser in Control Theory in the Classroom (1986) and The Quality School (1990). Glasser argues that for people to succeed at life in general, they must first experience success in one important aspect of their lives. For most children, that one important part should be school. But the traditional approach to evaluating learning, which emphasizes comparative grading (commonly called "grading on the curve"), allows only a minority of students to achieve A's and B's and feel successful. The self-worth of the remaining students (who may be quite capable) suffers, which depresses their motivation to achieve on subsequent classroom tasks (Covington, 1985).
Top
The Humanistic View of Motivation
Abraham Maslow earned his Ph.D. in a psychology department that supported the behaviorist position. After he graduated, however, he came into contact with Gestalt psychologists (a group of German psychologists whose work during the 1920s and 1930s laid the foundation for the cognitive theories of the 1960s and 1970s), prepared for a career as a psychoanalyst, and became interested in anthropology. As a result of these various influences, he came to the conclusion that American psychologists who endorsed the behaviorist position had become so preoccupied with overt behavior and objectivity that they were ignoring other important aspects of human existence (hence the term humanistic to describe his views). When Maslow observed the behavior of especially well-adjusted persons--or self-actualizers, as he called them--he concluded that healthy individuals are motivated to seek fulfilling experiences.
Maslow's Theory of Growth Motivation
Maslow describes seventeen propositions, discussed in Chapter 1 of Motivation and Personality (3d ed., 1987), that he believes would have to be incorporated into any sound theory of growth motivation (or need gratification) to meet them. Referring to need gratification as the most important single principle underlying all development, he adds that "the single, holistic principle that binds together the multiplicity of human motives is the tendency for a new and higher need to emerge as the lower need fulfills itself by being sufficiently gratified" (1968, p. 55). He elaborates on this basic principle by proposing a five-level hierarchy of needs. Physiological needs are at the bottom of the hierarchy, followed in ascending order by safety, belongingness and love, esteem, and self-actualization needs. This order reflects differences in the relative strength of each need. The lower a need is in the hierarchy, the greater is its strength because when a lower-level need is activated (as in the case of extreme hunger or fear for one's physical safety), people will stop trying to satisfy a higher-level need (such as esteem or self-actualization) and focus on satisfying the currently active lower-level need (Maslow, 1987).
The first four needs (physiological, safety, belongingness and love, and esteem) are often referred to as deficiency needs because they motivate people to act only when they are unmet to some degree. Self-actualization, by contrast, is often called a growth need because people constantly strive to satisfy it. Basically, self-actualization refers to the need for self-fulfillment -- the need to develop all of one's potential talents and capabilities. For example, an individual who felt she had the capability to write novels, teach, practice medicine, and raise children would not feel self-actualized until all of these goals had been accomplished to some minimal degree. Because it is at the top of the hierarchy and addresses the potential of the whole person, self-actualization is discussed more frequently than the other needs.
Maslow originally felt that self-actualization needs would automatically be activated as soon as esteem needs were met, but he changed his mind when he encountered individuals whose behavior did not fit this pattern. He concluded that individuals whose self-actualization needs became activated held in high regard such values as truth, goodness, beauty, justice, autonomy, and humor (Feist, 1990).
In addition to the five basic needs that compose the hierarchy, Maslow describes cognitive needs (such as the needs to know and to understand) and aesthetic needs (such as the needs for order, symmetry, or harmony). While not part of the basic hierarchy, these two classes of needs play a critical role in the satisfaction of basic needs. Maslow maintains that such conditions as the freedom to investigate and learn, fairness, honesty, and orderliness in interpersonal relationships are critical because their absence makes satisfaction of the five basic needs impossible. (Imagine, for example, trying to satisfy your belongingness and love needs or your esteem needs in an atmosphere characterized by dishonesty, unfair punishment, and restrictions on freedom of speech.)
Top
The Impact of Cooperative Learning on Motivation
Classroom tasks can be structured so that students are forced to compete with one another, work individually, or cooperate with one another to obtain the rewards that teachers make available for successfully completing these tasks. Traditionally, competitive arrangements have been assumed to be superior to the other two in increasing motivation and learning. But reviews of the research literature by David Johnson and Roger Johnson (Johnson & Johnson, 1995; Johnson, Johnson, & Smith, 1995) found cooperative arrangements to be far superior in producing these benefits. In this section we will describe cooperative-, competitive, and individual learning arrangements (sometimes called goal structures or reward structures), identify the elements that make up the major approaches to cooperative learning, and examine the effect of cooperative learning on motivation, achievement, and interpersonal relationships.
Types of Classroom Reward Structures
Competitive goal structures are typically norm referenced. (If you can't recall our discussion of the normal curve in Chapter 5, now might be a good time for a quick review.) This traditional practice of grading on the curve predetermines the percentage of A, B, C, D, and F grades regardless of the actual distribution of test scores. Because only a small percentage of students in any group can achieve the highest rewards and because this accomplishment must come at some other students' expense, competitive goal structures are characterized by negative interdependence. Students try to outdo one another, view classmates' failures as an advantage, and come to believe that the winners deserve their rewards because they are inherently better (Johnson, Johnson, & Holubec, 1994; Johnson et al., 1995).
Some researchers have argued that competitive reward structures lead students to focus on ability as the primary basis for motivation. This orientation is reflected in the question "Am I smart enough to accomplish this task?" When ability is the basis for motivation, competing successfully in the classroom may be seen as relevant to self-esteem (since nobody loves a loser), difficult to accomplish (since only a few can succeed), and uncertain (success depends on how everyone else does). These perceptions may cause some students to avoid challenging subjects or tasks, to give up in the face of difficulty, to reward themselves only if they win a competition, and to believe that their own successes are due to ability, whereas the successes of others are due to luck (Ames & Ames, 1984; Dweck, 1986).
Individualistic goal structures are characterized by students working alone and earning rewards solely on the quality of their own efforts. The success or failure of other students is irrelevant. All that matters is whether the student meets the standards for a particular task (Johnson et al., 1994; Johnson et al., 1995). Thirty students working by themselves at computer terminals are functioning in an individual reward structure. According to Carole Ames and Russell Ames (1984), individual structures lead students to focus on task effort as the primary basis for motivation (as in "I can do this if I try"). Whether a student perceives a task as difficult depends on how successful she has been with that type of task in the past.
Cooperative goal structures are characterized by students working together to accomplish shared goals. What is beneficial for the other students in the group is beneficial for the individual and vice versa. Because students in cooperative groups can obtain a desired reward (such as a high grade or a feeling of satisfaction for a job well done) only if the other students in the group also obtain the same reward, cooperative goal structures are characterized by positive interdependence. Also, all groups may receive the same rewards, provided they meet the teacher's criteria for mastery. For example, a teacher might present a lesson on map reading, then give each group its own map and a question-answering exercise. Students then work with each other to ensure that all know how to interpret maps. Each student then takes a quiz on map reading. All teams whose average quiz scores meet a preset standard receive special recognition (Johnson et al., 1994; Johnson et al., 1995; Slavin, 1995).
Cooperative structures lead students to focus on effort and cooperation as the primary basis of motivation. This orientation is reflected in the statement "We can do this if we try hard and work together." In a cooperative atmosphere, students are motivated out of a sense of obligation: one ought to try, contribute, and help satisfy group norms (Ames & Ames, 1984). William Glasser, whose ideas we mentioned earlier, is a fan of cooperative learning. He points out that student motivation and performance tend to be highest for such activities as band, drama club, athletics, the school newspaper, and the yearbook, all of which require a team effort (Gough, 1987). We would also like to point out that cooperative-learning and reward structures are consistent with the constructivist approach discussed in Chapters 1, 2, and 10 since they encourage inquiry, perspective sharing, and conflict resolution.
Top
Suggestions for Teaching in Your Classroom: Motivating Students to Learn
1. Use behavioral techniques to help students exert themselves and work toward remote goals.
2. Make sure that students know what they are to do, how to proceed, and how to determine when they have achieved goals.
3. Do everything possible to satisfy deficiency needs -- physiological, safety, belongingness, and esteem.
a. Accommodate the instructional program to the physiological needs of your students.
b. Make your room physically and psychologically safe.
c. Show your students that you take an interest in them and that they belong in your classroom.
d. Arrange learning experiences so that all students can gain at least a degree of esteem.
4. Enhance the attractions and minimize the dangers of growth choices.
5. Direct learning experiences toward feelings of success in an effort to encourage an orientation toward achievement, a positive self-concept, and a strong sense of self-efficacy.
a. Make use of objectives that are challenging but attainable and, when appropriate, that involve student input.
b. Provide knowledge of results by emphasizing the positive.
6. Try to encourage the development of need achievement, self-confidence, and self-direction in students who need these qualities.
a. Use achievement-motivation training techniques.
b. Use cooperative-learning methods.
7. Try to make learning interesting by emphasizing activity, investigation, adventure, social interaction, and usefulness.
Top
Resources for Further Investigation
Surveys of Motivational Theories
In a basic survey text, Motivation to Learn: From Theory to Practice (2d ed., 1993), Deborah Stipek discusses reinforcement theory, social cognitive theory, intrinsic motivation, need for achievement theory, attribution theory, and perceptions of ability. In Appendix 2-A, she presents a rating form and scoring procedure with which teachers can identify students who may have motivation problems. Appendix 3-A is a self-rating form that teachers can use to keep track of how often they provide rewards and punishments.
A useful summary of motivation theories and techniques can be found in the Worcester Polytechnic University's WWW site for teacher development, at http://www.wpi.edu/~isg_501/motivation.html.
Top
Motivational Techniques for the Classroom
Motivation and Teaching: A Practical Guide (1978), by Raymond Wlodkowski, and Eager to Learn (1990), by Raymond Wlodkowski and Judith Jaynes, are a good source of classroom application ideas. Motivating Students to Learn: Overcoming Barriers to High Achievement (1993), edited by Tommy Tomlinson, devotes four chapters to elementary school and four chapters to high school motivation issues.
Two sources of information on motivation techniques and suggestions for teaching are found at Columbia University's Institute for Learning Technologies, which contains documents, papers, and unusual projects and activities that could be used to increase student motivation; and at Northwestern University's Institute for Learning Sciences Engines for Education on-line program, which allows educators to pursue a number of questions about students, learning environments, and successful teaching through a hyperlinked database. The Institute for Learning Technologies is found at http://www.ilt.columbia.edu/ilt/. The Institute for Learning Sciences is found at http://www.ils.nwu.edu/.
For more information on "Motivation" in Gage/Berliner, EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, 6/e, Houghton Mifflin Co., 1998, see Chapter 8, "Motivation and Learning"
For more information on "Motivation" in the Grabes' INTEGRATING TECHNOLOGY FOR MEANINGFUL LEARNING, 2/e, Houghton Mifflin Co., 1998 see page 97 for "the role of motivation in drill and practice," pages 51-55 for "the role of motivation in meaningful learning", page 163 for "the role of motivativation in writing," and pages 398-99 for "learning styles and social and motivational preferences."
See the article here:
Teaching Concepts: Motivation Cengage Learning
What Is Motivation Motivation
Posted: at 5:51 am
%PDF-1.3 % 541 0 obj << /Linearized 1 /O 544 /H [ 6060 1504 ] /L 467640 /E 220226 /N 6 /T 456701 >> endobj xref 541 261 0000000016 00000 n 0000005572 00000 n 0000005938 00000 n 0000007564 00000 n 0000010282 00000 n 0000010867 00000 n 0000011097 00000 n 0000011147 00000 n 0000011177 00000 n 0000011218 00000 n 0000011444 00000 n 0000011985 00000 n 0000012009 00000 n 0000022979 00000 n 0000023456 00000 n 0000023692 00000 n 0000023715 00000 n 0000033772 00000 n 0000033795 00000 n 0000043350 00000 n 0000043373 00000 n 0000052944 00000 n 0000052968 00000 n 0000063292 00000 n 0000063316 00000 n 0000077312 00000 n 0000077336 00000 n 0000090562 00000 n 0000090742 00000 n 0000090766 00000 n 0000104571 00000 n 0000104747 00000 n 0000104890 00000 n 0000105069 00000 n 0000105212 00000 n 0000105382 00000 n 0000105547 00000 n 0000105717 00000 n 0000105887 00000 n 0000106057 00000 n 0000106230 00000 n 0000106373 00000 n 0000106516 00000 n 0000106680 00000 n 0000106823 00000 n 0000106985 00000 n 0000107149 00000 n 0000107292 00000 n 0000107432 00000 n 0000107594 00000 n 0000107764 00000 n 0000107907 00000 n 0000108056 00000 n 0000108199 00000 n 0000108367 00000 n 0000108496 00000 n 0000108633 00000 n 0000108803 00000 n 0000108936 00000 n 0000109079 00000 n 0000109249 00000 n 0000109411 00000 n 0000109573 00000 n 0000109737 00000 n 0000109907 00000 n 0000110077 00000 n 0000110247 00000 n 0000110417 00000 n 0000110587 00000 n 0000110757 00000 n 0000110920 00000 n 0000111059 00000 n 0000111234 00000 n 0000111367 00000 n 0000111516 00000 n 0000111682 00000 n 0000111839 00000 n 0000111985 00000 n 0000112114 00000 n 0000114792 00000 n 0000139864 00000 n 0000156470 00000 n 0000189420 00000 n 0000189476 00000 n 0000189651 00000 n 0000189785 00000 n 0000189864 00000 n 0000190071 00000 n 0000190237 00000 n 0000190407 00000 n 0000190556 00000 n 0000190699 00000 n 0000190869 00000 n 0000191018 00000 n 0000191161 00000 n 0000191331 00000 n 0000191501 00000 n 0000191650 00000 n 0000191820 00000 n 0000191980 00000 n 0000192129 00000 n 0000192284 00000 n 0000192450 00000 n 0000192599 00000 n 0000192739 00000 n 0000192909 00000 n 0000193077 00000 n 0000193226 00000 n 0000193389 00000 n 0000193538 00000 n 0000193705 00000 n 0000193881 00000 n 0000194030 00000 n 0000194197 00000 n 0000194356 00000 n 0000194517 00000 n 0000194684 00000 n 0000194851 00000 n 0000195025 00000 n 0000195198 00000 n 0000195365 00000 n 0000195508 00000 n 0000195684 00000 n 0000195849 00000 n 0000195989 00000 n 0000196138 00000 n 0000196314 00000 n 0000196490 00000 n 0000196666 00000 n 0000196825 00000 n 0000196982 00000 n 0000197137 00000 n 0000197280 00000 n 0000197453 00000 n 0000197608 00000 n 0000197751 00000 n 0000197918 00000 n 0000198055 00000 n 0000198210 00000 n 0000198365 00000 n 0000198524 00000 n 0000198691 00000 n 0000198858 00000 n 0000199019 00000 n 0000199180 00000 n 0000199329 00000 n 0000199490 00000 n 0000199649 00000 n 0000199792 00000 n 0000199935 00000 n 0000200078 00000 n 0000200254 00000 n 0000200417 00000 n 0000200599 00000 n 0000200775 00000 n 0000200951 00000 n 0000201118 00000 n 0000201285 00000 n 0000201450 00000 n 0000201623 00000 n 0000201793 00000 n 0000201969 00000 n 0000202135 00000 n 0000202307 00000 n 0000202479 00000 n 0000202651 00000 n 0000202825 00000 n 0000203001 00000 n 0000203177 00000 n 0000203353 00000 n 0000203529 00000 n 0000203705 00000 n 0000203872 00000 n 0000204021 00000 n 0000204176 00000 n 0000204341 00000 n 0000204508 00000 n 0000204657 00000 n 0000204827 00000 n 0000204970 00000 n 0000205119 00000 n 0000205268 00000 n 0000205438 00000 n 0000205605 00000 n 0000205766 00000 n 0000205925 00000 n 0000206092 00000 n 0000206251 00000 n 0000206418 00000 n 0000206585 00000 n 0000206752 00000 n 0000206919 00000 n 0000207086 00000 n 0000207253 00000 n 0000207399 00000 n 0000207569 00000 n 0000207739 00000 n 0000207878 00000 n 0000208057 00000 n 0000208186 00000 n 0000208359 00000 n 0000208529 00000 n 0000208693 00000 n 0000208855 00000 n 0000209017 00000 n 0000209151 00000 n 0000209314 00000 n 0000209440 00000 n 0000209603 00000 n 0000209752 00000 n 0000209922 00000 n 0000210049 00000 n 0000210207 00000 n 0000210330 00000 n 0000210482 00000 n 0000210613 00000 n 0000210777 00000 n 0000210900 00000 n 0000211067 00000 n 0000211194 00000 n 0000211361 00000 n 0000211494 00000 n 0000211655 00000 n 0000211794 00000 n 0000211967 00000 n 0000212116 00000 n 0000212286 00000 n 0000212456 00000 n 0000212632 00000 n 0000212808 00000 n 0000212984 00000 n 0000213158 00000 n 0000213332 00000 n 0000213508 00000 n 0000213684 00000 n 0000213860 00000 n 0000214030 00000 n 0000214200 00000 n 0000214368 00000 n 0000214536 00000 n 0000214696 00000 n 0000214856 00000 n 0000215016 00000 n 0000215179 00000 n 0000215322 00000 n 0000215485 00000 n 0000215614 00000 n 0000215777 00000 n 0000215926 00000 n 0000216083 00000 n 0000216232 00000 n 0000216395 00000 n 0000216544 00000 n 0000216707 00000 n 0000216856 00000 n 0000217019 00000 n 0000217168 00000 n 0000217333 00000 n 0000217503 00000 n 0000006060 00000 n 0000007541 00000 n trailer << /Size 802 /Info 539 0 R /Root 542 0 R /Prev 456690 /ID[<9285a78fe4cc240cf43549f8dfca0e9c>] >> startxref 0 %%EOF 542 0 obj << /Type /Catalog /Pages 538 0 R /Metadata 540 0 R /Outlines 521 0 R /OpenAction [ 544 0 R /Fit ] /PageMode /UseNone /PageLayout /SinglePage /PageLabels 537 0 R /StructTreeRoot 543 0 R /PieceInfo << /MarkedPDF << /LastModified (D:20020926151625)>> >> /LastModified (D:20020926151625) /MarkInfo << /Marked true /LetterspaceFlags 0 >> >> endobj 543 0 obj << /Type /StructTreeRoot /K [ 529 0 R 530 0 R 531 0 R 532 0 R 533 0 R 534 0 R ] /RoleMap 535 0 R >> endobj 800 0 obj << /S 1531 /O 2223 /L 2239 /C 2255 /Filter /FlateDecode /Length 801 0 R >> stream HV{LW?m#-(J>X%e@J'!lHKZZ(SDbA,8sK.[lUo{;{|Gx?{3"^pBZ AkOoFl0Gxp^ $=y~,drapHe?];-]3T 3o):ejU,JxTZ|':{WJ o ww$4Y~dFv5DJi|(}kGJD[iy_rbvof?>wP_2<7zG_ Q{{/E}%2h61[)l#8x3]|#'6 <3r;`*sy+nL^E~jHr5;2u-cZG*-j?vdP8[2aY9'1~K7)`HA<$|`t|)Jn!89C+oe{2D7m8!O9{XOSCdVFuG_q%=Q/~fVcu|C&(L3 t9i2A)m'&tU=iHI/2dtgONN 5{V} q|X:o(ZWM /*{i/Tx@YD<'8?H2%k/YTk 9*(+(3p L_ @YU&(1BtLzEkB@5BTh!0$:}U@h`Tp}iH&h} g< R70-jN7 -@""up"cqtbyZI?a $9!Q dmva2r&(F(b(GD|^AF;Z^p0u0be!hYV2;jHqF(miP4~^b7ZJCqF/p!f]iH{7}MeN25 2pt3o endstream endobj 801 0 obj 1369 endobj 544 0 obj << /Type /Page /Parent 538 0 R /Resources << /ColorSpace << /CS3 549 0 R /CS4 548 0 R /CS5 547 0 R >> /ExtGState << /GS2 626 0 R /GS3 627 0 R >> /Font << /TT3 551 0 R /TT4 545 0 R /TT5 554 0 R >> /Shading << /Sh149 624 0 R /Sh150 612 0 R /Sh151 610 0 R /Sh152 616 0 R /Sh153 615 0 R /Sh154 628 0 R /Sh155 642 0 R /Sh156 639 0 R /Sh157 646 0 R /Sh158 645 0 R /Sh159 638 0 R /Sh160 632 0 R /Sh161 629 0 R /Sh162 609 0 R /Sh163 635 0 R /Sh164 636 0 R /Sh165 583 0 R /Sh166 585 0 R /Sh167 589 0 R /Sh168 586 0 R /Sh169 735 0 R /Sh170 736 0 R /Sh171 575 0 R /Sh172 571 0 R /Sh173 573 0 R /Sh174 738 0 R /Sh175 740 0 R /Sh176 580 0 R /Sh177 576 0 R /Sh178 578 0 R /Sh179 577 0 R /Sh180 579 0 R /Sh181 601 0 R /Sh182 602 0 R /Sh183 603 0 R /Sh184 590 0 R /Sh185 600 0 R /Sh186 604 0 R /Sh187 607 0 R /Sh188 608 0 R /Sh189 606 0 R /Sh190 605 0 R /Sh191 741 0 R /Sh192 742 0 R /Sh193 743 0 R /Sh194 744 0 R /Sh195 594 0 R /Sh196 597 0 R /Sh197 746 0 R /Sh198 748 0 R /Sh199 648 0 R /Sh200 670 0 R /Sh201 704 0 R /Sh202 705 0 R /Sh203 703 0 R /Sh204 702 0 R /Sh205 706 0 R /Sh206 710 0 R /Sh207 711 0 R /Sh208 709 0 R /Sh209 707 0 R /Sh210 708 0 R /Sh211 701 0 R /Sh212 694 0 R /Sh213 750 0 R /Sh214 722 0 R /Sh215 718 0 R /Sh216 691 0 R /Sh217 662 0 R /Sh218 695 0 R /Sh219 693 0 R /Sh220 752 0 R /Sh221 754 0 R /Sh222 756 0 R /Sh223 758 0 R /Sh224 760 0 R /Sh225 762 0 R /Sh226 660 0 R /Sh227 764 0 R /Sh228 659 0 R /Sh229 663 0 R /Sh230 667 0 R /Sh231 668 0 R /Sh232 666 0 R /Sh233 766 0 R /Sh234 767 0 R /Sh235 768 0 R /Sh236 769 0 R /Sh237 770 0 R /Sh238 771 0 R /Sh239 772 0 R /Sh240 773 0 R /Sh241 774 0 R /Sh242 775 0 R /Sh243 776 0 R /Sh244 777 0 R /Sh245 778 0 R /Sh246 779 0 R /Sh247 658 0 R /Sh248 651 0 R /Sh249 780 0 R /Sh250 781 0 R /Sh251 782 0 R /Sh252 783 0 R /Sh253 785 0 R /Sh254 787 0 R /Sh255 692 0 R /Sh256 789 0 R /Sh257 791 0 R /Sh258 793 0 R /Sh259 795 0 R /Sh260 797 0 R /Sh261 650 0 R /Sh262 653 0 R /Sh263 656 0 R /Sh264 657 0 R /Sh265 655 0 R /Sh266 654 0 R /Sh267 669 0 R /Sh268 683 0 R /Sh269 684 0 R /Sh270 682 0 R /Sh271 681 0 R /Sh272 696 0 R /Sh273 699 0 R /Sh274 700 0 R /Sh275 798 0 R /Sh276 799 0 R /Sh277 673 0 R /Sh278 698 0 R /Sh279 697 0 R /Sh280 712 0 R /Sh281 726 0 R /Sh282 727 0 R /Sh283 725 0 R /Sh284 724 0 R /Sh285 728 0 R /Sh286 732 0 R /Sh287 733 0 R /Sh288 731 0 R /Sh289 729 0 R /Sh290 730 0 R /Sh291 723 0 R /Sh292 676 0 R /Sh293 686 0 R /Sh294 687 0 R /Sh295 680 0 R /Sh296 715 0 R /Sh297 716 0 R >> /XObject << /Im1 568 0 R >> /ProcSet [ /PDF /Text /ImageC /ImageI ] >> /Contents [ 553 0 R 557 0 R 559 0 R 561 0 R 563 0 R 565 0 R 567 0 R 570 0 R ] /MediaBox [ 0 0 612 792 ] /CropBox [ 37 37 575 755 ] /Rotate 0 >> endobj 545 0 obj << /Type /Font /Subtype /TrueType /FirstChar 32 /LastChar 150 /Widths [ 250 333 0 0 500 0 0 0 333 333 0 0 250 333 250 278 500 500 500 500 500 0 500 500 500 500 278 278 564 564 564 0 0 722 667 667 722 611 556 722 722 333 389 722 611 889 722 722 556 722 667 556 611 722 722 944 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 444 500 444 500 444 333 500 500 278 278 500 278 778 500 500 500 500 333 389 278 500 500 722 500 500 444 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 333 444 444 350 500 ] /Encoding /WinAnsiEncoding /BaseFont /AOADND+TimesNewRoman /FontDescriptor 546 0 R >> endobj 546 0 obj << /Type /FontDescriptor /Ascent 891 /CapHeight 656 /Descent -216 /Flags 34 /FontBBox [ -568 -307 2028 1007 ] /FontName /AOADND+TimesNewRoman /ItalicAngle 0 /StemV 94 /XHeight 0 /FontFile2 622 0 R >> endobj 547 0 obj [ /Indexed 549 0 R 0 623 0 R ] endobj 548 0 obj /DeviceGray endobj 549 0 obj [ /ICCBased 619 0 R ] endobj 550 0 obj << /Type /FontDescriptor /Ascent 1000 /CapHeight 734 /Descent -206 /Flags 32 /FontBBox [ -600 -208 1338 1034 ] /FontName /AOACFL+Tahoma /ItalicAngle 0 /StemV 96 /XHeight 546 /FontFile2 621 0 R >> endobj 551 0 obj << /Type /Font /Subtype /TrueType /FirstChar 32 /LastChar 151 /Widths [ 313 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 383 383 0 0 303 363 303 0 0 546 546 546 546 546 546 546 0 546 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 600 589 601 678 561 0 667 0 373 417 0 498 771 0 708 551 0 621 557 584 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 525 553 461 553 526 318 553 558 229 0 498 229 840 558 543 553 0 360 446 334 558 498 742 495 498 444 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 211 401 401 0 546 909 ] /Encoding /WinAnsiEncoding /BaseFont /AOACFL+Tahoma /FontDescriptor 550 0 R >> endobj 552 0 obj 10890 endobj 553 0 obj << /Filter /FlateDecode /Length 552 0 R >> stream HME+L"`(F6YD6`x!(#iA&HG:0t[e-}7o_<7>yyz4}yy_/p//?aQotxe4]8OOoOw? /w~7 7|~{7.:u-]zWUO?nppCO@[O#.`&O0,Z9^%_O7/2=oT7b{#Q,1%o/F}k~:<~R}g.w+r{6c5xOgg[?mZ@=7{ui%o$u~ |co-iE@-c5K>r*_*J~:|~xtxl-@9e?'v MA0OAGhvSS=WVHJuu+TE3[1HpIeV4n0JSc9'fCVuUf:9tM |nmYxs ~#%!,jy!)KScP#rbGJ B$jSG_|ty=zb Rjj/.%IaJdmj]Jo$]JrRr&{v.%R|.%sM #20'mPllS6%tbopdpcSc20%kSG R9]Jjv.9t.jf` RI]Jcj g VyU?:>w5LIhS:WMUML6 rhS8=1aO3'M |SA~tM }Oy158hjrQy6*,'v/z>r OM >r=[ @>UF!'~ u5>7NV~jj>*ES W91;utIMS- 9=N)l%n}HNn0.bsUtbkSO+GO>75x~JELNMu<~>u|W)Ym*$W8_daV4dj8=1M ]10'WEzO.hSkoS-1Qk%nKefRd
T d)P}PM[dD@ 0@ c_>@}5t `0-28- aZ8-]ipZ8iPh`<,E>BsMR 0xHgiBOr@:Knyx|Q[O)k f+uOF{O"bSk4@8w@&: Hw$"JV4.Lp`j:C{ )Y|f.wsZYNs_s=='&ggtp]mT+"=WFK9!uU},IKaXshh"d}jsq,EV @xpj/c~q5YM}v_aC!ol%eZGIU`57T_A:dKj*w8RKM~^UPy4(be*zEtx`[-N+@mCkDr@)nQ:d"kTjx'"u)5+~j_%?"yBU+TV> 0j|u~ ;X|a.u-]k-jFA,lM&{Zx"}-[]Bx' -[,Rq-j6w3;O.Y=@PRfv//]j5T9PfX{geK{+7*.iw$X|@wf,KS0tG]u%{[YW8?A;|pj;PCE_ Eg^aSX8-]b-brm-[l}Z}k>yd5W$emB{|rXm^GGnk],^s[USa/ipmk_,?B//Z%9 V09[8@i2o*c2 %>Z7/5